Historians agree that behaviorism was the dominant force
in the creation of modern American psychology.1 Now that psychology
has returned to the eclecticism of its earlier years, we can analyze
behaviorism’s role in American psychology. Yet scholars of behaviorism
stand face to face with a paradox. It would appear that we know
everything we could possibly want to know about behaviorism, but
behaviorism and its role in psychology remain mysterious and enigmatic.
We know everything about behaviorism because behaviorists
themselves have written numerous accounts of behaviorism in general
as well as of various specific aspects of it, because we have a surfeit of
secondary accounts of behaviorism and of behaviorist theories, and
because we have volumes of critical writing on behaviorism. Even so,
behaviorism remains an enigma because its dominance in American
psychology blocks our efforts to understand its role and its nature.
American psychologists (and many outside the United States or
Canada, especially in the English-speaking world) are trained to think
behavioristically from their earliest undergraduate years, usually
without being made aware, or realizing, that this is the case. A truly
committed and highly trained American psychologist who strives to
articulate the fundamental elements of his or her research practices
will state a set of behaviorist propositions because it is the academic
culture of behaviorism that will dictate the seemingly self-evident
basis of the psychological enterprise.